Los Avances En La Investigación De La
Integración Sensorial
La teoría de integración sensorial, que se derivó principalmente
de los campos de la medicina, la neurología, y el desarrollo infantil,
trata de explicar las relaciones entre los buques de procesos
neurológicos y conductas manifiestas (Ayres, 1972b). La teoría de
integración sensorial tiene como objetivo aumentar nuestra
comprensión de las causas subyacentes de los problemas de conducta,
dificultades motoras, problemas de aprendizaje y de algunos niños. Como marco de referencia, la
integración sensorial actúa como
puente entre la teoría y la practica clínica,
proporcionando un conjunto de directrices para la evaluación e
intervención. Estas directrices son una herramienta
integral para la promoción de la investigación.
La integración sensorial ha sido objeto de una investigación
mas que cualquier otro enfoque o marco de
referencia dentro del
campo de la terapia ocupacional (Miller & Kinnealy, 1993; Parpham y
Mailloux, 2001). Durante muchos años, ha sido uno de los marcos mas aplicadas de referencia utilizado por
terapeutas ocupacionales pediatricos. A pesar de esta popularidad, la
investigación hasta la fecha no se ha establecido claramente el consenso
profesional con respecto a la simple pregunta: ¿la intervención
efectiva de la integración sensorial?
La integración sensorial ha sido criticado, sobre todo en la literatura
educativa, donde la eficacia se ha desestimado por muchos investigadores (por
ejemplo, de Hoehn y Baumeister, 1994). La legitimidad científica para este enfoque tiene, por tanto, no ha establecido
todavía.
Objeto y Ambito
El propósito de este capítulo es
examinar lavalidez de la integración sensorial, tanto como una teoría y un marco de
referencia. Para llevar a cabo esta tarea, revisar y sintetizar la literatura
mas reciente (a partir de aproximadamente 1986 a 2000) de los
métodos basicos y aplicados de la investigación
científica. También se discuten las formas en que los
profesionales que seleccionan la integración sensorial como un
método de intervención pueden aplicarse esta
investigación. Llegamos a la conclusión de sugerir direcciones para
futuras investigaciones para establecer un consenso profesional, tanto dentro
como fuera de la terapia ocupacional con respecto a la validez de la
teoría de integración sensorial y su aplicación como un
enfoque de intervención.
Investigación Examinar La Validez De La
Integración Sensorial
Validación de la teoría de integración sensorial de la
integración de la información y el conocimiento de los
métodos basicos y aplicados de la investigación
científica. La investigación basica trata de
responder a preguntas fundamentales sobre la naturaleza de la conducta. Las
preguntas de investigación, tales como
'¿Qué es la integración sensorial?',
'¿Qué es la disfunción de integración
sensorial? Y '¿Por qué
funciona la integración sensorial intervención?' son ejemplos
que caen bajo
esta categoría. Ademas, la investigación
basica analiza las muchas teorías sobre la cual se deriva la
integración sensorial, incluyendo los postulados que lo apoyan.
La investigación aplicada es la investigación científica
que directamente los intentos de resolver problemas practicos. Ejemplos
de investigación aplicada en el ambito de la integración
sensorial son los estudios que examinan la eficacia de laintervención de
la integración sensorial, los estudios que tienen como objetivo desarrollar
o evaluar los instrumentos de evaluación de la medición de las
funciones de integración sensorial, y los informes de casos que
describen la intervención y los cambios funcionales que se derivan de la
intervención. La investigación es sobre la búsqueda del
conocimiento y el desarrollo de formas de pensar, organizar y aplicar lo que
sabemos. Sin embargo, los hallazgos científicos y las teorías son
siempre considerados como provisionales.
Independientemente de la forma elocuente una teoría en particular ha
sido probada, siempre se considera tentativa - acercando mas y
mas hacia la 'verdad', pero nunca lograr ese
fin.
Sensory integration theory and its application as a means of evaluating and
intervening with children with certain disabilities have continued to evolve
since its inception in the early 1960s. This evolution has occurred in response
to research findings over time as well as to the practical demands placed on
the professionals who practice within this framework. Intervention based on sensory
integration theory is very complex, as are the clients best suited for this
approach and the theory on which it is based. Therefore, whether a researcher
is interested in conducting basic or applied scientific inquiry, complexity
provides a significant challenge.
To establish professional consensus regarding the validation of sensory
integration theory, evaluation and support for the basic assumptions on which
the theory is based are necessary. A clear conceptualization of what sensory
integration is, and what constitutes sensory integration intervention is
crucial. In addiction, efficacystudies are necessary to determine whether the
intervention is effective, assist in identifying, and identify the functional
areas affected by intervention. Finally, efficacy studies must evaluate the
intervention in the ways and contexts in which practitioners are applying it in
order for the research to be most useful.
We begin by examining research that explores the concept of sensory
integration. We emphasize research examining sensory integration intervention
as an approach that focuses on remediating underlying etiology or neurologic
dysfunction rather than on direct instruction or skill development. We have
also included a review of research on central nervous system (CNS) plasticity
and hierarchy, two important assumptions supporting sensory processing is
discussed. Second, we discuss research examining the concept of sensory
integrative dysfunction. Third, we present efficacy studies evaluating the
outcomes of sensory integration interventions. Finally, we conclude by
discussing challenges related to research in the area of sensory integration
and include ideas and priorities for furthering our knowledge in this area.
Sensory Integration: A Process Approach
In the educational literature, a process approach focuses or remediating
underlying neurologic on mental processes believed to be contributing to a
child’s inability to learn or perform a specific skill as opposed to
approaches that directly teach skills. In the late 1960s an 1970s, a number of
process approaches, including sensory integration, perceptual- motor (Frostig,
1967; Kephart, 1971), and neurodevelopment therapy (Bobath, 1980) were popalla
with practitioners intervening with children withneurologic disorders (e.g.,
learning disabilities and cerebral palsy). Process approaches are based on the
belief that the functioning of specific neurologic systems (i.e., those
responsible for sensory processing, motor coordination, and sustaining
attention was required for adequate cognitive development. After the disordered
underlying processes were corrected, academic learning could take place
normality. (Hammill, 1993).
Sensory integration con be viewed as a process
approach. Through the provision of sensory experiences, within the context of
meaningful activity, and the production of adaptive responses, CNS functioning
improves. This improvement ultimately leads to better performance in any number
of functional areas, including behavior, academics, and motor skill (Fisher
Munrray, 1991). However, in the mid-1970s and 1980s, researchers (Goodman &
Hammill ; Kavale & Mattson, 1983) conducting
efficacy studies evaluating process-oriented inventions in education concluded
that these approaches were largely ineffective. This is began
to question whether underlying neurologic processes were related to cognitive
abilities or academic performance. Eventually, on- task, direct instruction
forms of assessment and remediation replaced process approaches. “For the
moment, the issue of process training is resolved, and direct instruction has
emerged as the model of choice for the remediation of learning disabilities in
the United States” (Hammill, 1993, p 303).
Sensory integration was not tested specifically in these studies, and most of
these studies were limited to the examination of process approaches with
children with learning disabilities. Nonetheless, thedescription of sensory
integration simply as a process approach has probably contributed to the
negative views of some researchers, particularly in the field of education.
Those who accept effectiveness of intervention based on sensory integration
theory believe first that CNS has the capacity for change or remediation.
Neuroplasticity is the assumption that the CNS has the capacity for change or
is able to modify its structure and function (Lenn, 1991). Second, proponents
of sensory integration theory believe in a certain level CNS hierarchy because
it is assumed that positive change or adaptation of lower (brainstem level) areas
may result in improved higher cortical functions, including any number of
functional areas and capabilities. Research related to these two basic
assumptions on which sensory integration is based is examined below.
Sensory Integration: A theory Based on Central Nervous System Plasticity and
Hierarchical Concepts.
There is an abundance of research that support the
concept of neuroplasticity (Bach –y –Rita, 1980; Leen, 1991; Lund,
1978; McEachen & Shaw, 1996; Stein et al., 1974; Stephenson, 1993; Szekely,
1979). Neuroplasticity is evident throughout development; it involves natural
changes in the nervous system that occur during maturation. It also is a
reactive process that occurs during the recovery form an injury to the CNS
(Schaaf, 1994a). Because the majority of individuals who are treated whit
sensory integration procedures are children with chronic, developmental
conditions (i.e., pervasive developmental disorders, attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder, learning disabilities) and because sensory integration
theory is not designed toexplain adult-onset deficits (see Chapter 1), research
that examines developmental neuroplasticity is most relevant.
Some of the most importante factors that promote developmental neuroplasticity
are the inner drive to seek, create, challenge, and master the environment
(Aoki & Siekevitz, 1998; Parham & Mailloux, 2001), the just –
right challenge are fundamental characteristics of intervention based on
sensory integration theory (Ayres, 1979) and may be keys to why it works.
Therefore, research that examines such factors provides us with helpful
guidelines neural organization through sensory integration intervention.
Neuroplasticity takes place through a number of specific neural mechanisms,
such as an increase in myelination and synaptic efficiency and dendritic
aborization. Explanations of such mechanisms are beyond the scope of this
chapter; however, Schaaf (1994a, 1994b) provided an overview of such mechanisms
and how they relate to our understanding of how and why intervention based on
sensory integration theory may be effective. Particularly relevant are the ways
in which we can enhance neural organization and integration through the types
of purposeful activities used during intervention.
Although the idea that a more integrated, more efficient CNS results in
improved performance in functional skills seems very logical, this issue has
been at the core of many debates. Unlike research that supports CNS plasticity
(which is very convincing), the connection between improved sensory processing
and functional skills and research examining brain function as a hierarchical
process provide useful information in examining this connection.
Case- Smith (1995)explored relationships among
sensorimotor components, fine motor skill, and functional performance in 30
preschool children. The sensorimotor components included two measures of
sensory processing. One of these, tactile defensiveness, correlated
significantly with measures of fine motor skills. However, the same measure
showed only a weak relationship to functional social, play, and self-care
skills. Case- Smith concluded that practitioners should not assume that
functional skills improve when gains are made in underlying sensorimotor
skills. Furthermore, other social, cultural, and environmental factors may be
just as important for children to learn, perform, and generalize functional
skills. She encouraged practitioners to consider such contexts in their
interventions with children.
Sensory integration largely focuses on the remediation of brainstem functions
as a way to improve functional skills (Ayres, 1972b). This hierarchical view
understands the CNS as having vertically arranged levels that are
interdependent yet reflect a trend of ascending control and specialization. This
hierarchical approach led Ayres (1972b) to believe that the more primitive or
subcortical systems such as the tactile, the vestibular, and the proprioceptive
systems provide the foundation for the development of higher-order cortical
functions such as academic ability, complex motor skills, and the development
of social skills. This relationship is, however, controversial because the
indirect correspondence between the stimulus (i.e., improved brainstem
functioning) and performance (e.g., motor skill, academic achievement) is, for
the most part, unobservable. More importantly, over the past10 to 20 years, a
more popular view of brain function, one that views the brain as a more
integrated, holistic system, has emerged (Cohen & Reed, 1996).
Sensory integration theory has evolved to incorporate this view, and today it
is consistent with a more holistic view, and today it is consistent with a more
holistic view of brain function. Fisher and Murray (1991) described sensory
integration theory as being based on a systems view, rather than viewing
sensory integration problems are believed to result from a number of
interrelated systems that are not functioning optimally. Each part of the
system performs a different role. Some are control centers; some fine-tune the instructions;
some carry feedback messages. The extent of give and take from each part
supports a holistic rather than a strictly hierarchical view.
The amount of literature that describes interventions that apply sensory
integration techniques in conjunction with other approaches has increased in
recent years. This literature suggests that sensory integrative dysfunction is
only one of many problems that contribute to a child’s difficulty in
performing or learning tasks effectively. Today, intervention based on sensory
integration theory is rarely provided isolation; it is more often combined with
other approaches, including the teaching of specific skills (Case- Smith, 1997)
and promoting positive play behaviors (see Chapter 15). Intervention activities
providing enhanced tactile and vestibular sensations are often used in
conjunction with activities that tap higher cortical systems such as those
involved in forming the idea about how to perform motor tasks and with practice
of specificfunctional skills in context (see Chapter 3).
A more holistic intervention approach does however,
dispute the emphasis that sensory integration has on the remediation of
primitive sensory systems or the importance of healthy sensory integration for
performing functional skills. Rather, in has placed this emphasis in context of
a more holistic and interactive view of brain function and question our ability
to separate subcortical from higher-cortical functions. Subsequently, the use
of other approaches in combination with intervention based on sensory
integration theory is encouraged (see also Chapter 1).
Sensory Integration: A Component of Sensory Processing
There has been some confusion related to the use of
terminology associated with sensory integration. The inconsistency in the use
of this terminology has made interpretation rather difficult. The term sensory
integration has been used to describe a nervous process (occurring at the
cellular or nervous system level), a behavioral process (observable behaviors
that result from these processes), and a frame of reference useful for
assessment and intervention. Thus, researchers must differentiating clearly
between what they observe in children and what they infer happening within the
nervous system. Miller and Lane (2000) addressed the meant to establish a
consensus for the use of various terms particularly those associated with
neurophysiological processes, including peripheral sensory processes and
central sensory processes (see also Chapter 4).
Sensory processing is one term that has because popular, and become it is often
used interchanges ably whit sensory integration, the differentiation between
these two termsrequires clarification. Both terms represent theoretical
frameworks for explaining the same types of functional deficits and behaviors
observed in children. However, sensory processing is a more global,
encompassing construct than is sensory integration. Sensory processing is the
way in which the central and peripheral nervous systems mange incoming sensory
information from tactile, vestibular, proprioceptive, visual, auditory,
olfactory, and gustatory sensory systems. According to Miller and Lane (2000),
the “reception, modulation, integration, and organization of sensory
stimuli, including the behavioral responses to the sensory input are all
components of sensory processing” (p. .
A conceptual model of sensory processing proposed by Dunn (1997) described the
relationships among a number of neurobiological factors, including sensory
registration (i.e., how one regulates incoming sensory information), and
habituation and sensitization (i.e., whether the CNS reacts to or ignores
incoming sensory information; see Chapter 7). In addition, Dunn attempted to
link sensory processing with the ways in ways individuals perform daily life
activities. Consistent with this more global use of the term sensory
processing, DeGangi (1991) reported that the sensory processing problems of
ported that the sensory processing problems of post-institutionalized children
often lead to problems with sensory integration. Therefore, sensory integration
should be viewed as only one component of sensory processing. Researchers must
make explicit their use of various terms, and consumers of research must attend
to the ways in which various terms are used.
RESEARCH EXMINING SENSORY INTEGRATIVEDYSFUNCTION
Sensory integrative dysfunction is a diagnostic label that is relatively
unpopular outside the profession of occupational therapy (Missiuna &
Polatajko, 1995), and it is not included in the Diagnostic and Statisyical
Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994.).
Rather, sensory integrative dysfunction is often associated with the underlying
sensory processing problems characteristic of any number of diagnostic
conditions included in the DSM-IV. The most common DSM-IV diagnostic groups
that coexist with sensory integrative dysfunction are pervasive developmental
disorders (e.g., autism) (Kientz & Dunn, 1997; McIntosh et al., 1999),
attention deficit-hyperactivity disorders (Mulligan, 1996), learning disorders
(Ayres, 1979), fragile X syndrome (Miler et al., 1999), and developmental
coordination disorder (Missiuna & Polatajko, 1995). Although not all
children in these diagnostic groups have evidence of symptoms associated with
it. Based on research to date, it also is not correct to assume causative
relationships between sensory integrative dysfunction and these other
conditions when the do coexist. The same underlying neurologic deficit may well
cause both conditions.
Sensory Integrative Dysfunction and Other Diagnostic Labels
The issue of classification of children has been debated over the years, and
the confusion of “labeling” has led to problems both in identifying
appropriate children for specific interventions and in selecting subjects for
research purposes. A review of four terms: sensory integrative dysfunction,
clumsy child syndrome, developmental dyspraxia, and developmental coordination
disorder byMissiuna & Polatajko (1995) concluded that these four terms should
not be used interchangeably and that clear definitions and characteristic
feactures of each need to identified.
Typically, children are labeled as having sensory integrative dysfunction when
they do poorly on tests specifically designed to measure sensory integration
(Missiuna & Polatajko, 1995). Such tests, such as the Sensory Integration
and Praxis Test ( SIPT; Ayres, 1989), measure the processing of basic sensory
systems (i.e., tactile, propioceptive, vestibular) as well as visual-perceptual
and motor planning functions. Specific types of sensory processing deficits are
beginning to be identified in children with different diagnostic
classifications. This research provides valuable information about the nature
of dysfunction characteristic of certain types of children. For example,
Mulligan (1996) indentified specific patterns of sensory integrative
dysfunction in children with attention disorders defensiveness specific to
children and adults with developmental disabilities. Kientz and Dunn (1997)
described the sensory processing of children with autism, and Demaio- Feldman
(1994) identified somatosensory processing deficiencies of low-birth-weight
infants at school age.
The Zero to Three Diagnostic Classification Task Force included a diagnostic
classification called multisystem developmental disorders in the Diagnostic
Classification of Mental Health and Developmental Disordesr of Infancy and
Early Childhood (DC 0-3) (as cited by Wieder, 1996). These multisystem
disorders are consistent with what has been described as characteristics of
sensory integrative dysfunction because they view avariety of functional
problems as being secondary processing deficits rather than as primary
deficits. The different types of multisystem disorders, however, place more
emphasis on sensory modulation (a component of sensory processing) than on
sensory integration. For example, the three types of multisystem developmental
disorders are a hypersensitive type (type 1), a hypersensitive type (type II),
and a motorically disorganized and impulsive type (type III). Research by
Miller and colleagues (personal communication, March 15, 2000) is exploring the
possibility of sensory modulation disorders as a separate diagnostic entity.
This research group has indentified and demonstrated types of sensory
modulation problems in children with autism and fragile X sindorme, using both
traditional measures and neurphysiological measures such as electrodermal
responses (McIntosh et al., 1999; Miller et al., 1999)
Types of Sensory Integrative Disorders
Ayres (1972s) believed that many different types of sensory integrative
disorders existed and that each was associated with dysfunction in a particular
neural substrate. She then developed a typology of sensory integrative
dysfunction based on a series of multivariate analyses (Ayres, 1989; see also
Chapter 1 and Appendix B). Sensory integrative dysfunction, therefore, has been
conceptualized as multidimensional. The identification of different types
of sensory integrative dysfunction assists with the understanding of underlying
etiology and, more practically, is useful for developing specific intervention
for the discrete patterns identified.
Categorical systems for conceptualizing sensory integrative dysfunction,
largely based on Ayres’factor analytic work in the 1960s and 1970s, are
described by Parham and Mailloux (2001), Fisher and Munrray (1991), and Ayres
(1989) (see also Chapter 1 and Appendix B). Although there is not perfect
consensus on the best way to categorize the patterns of dysfunction, there are
recurring themes, and much commonality expressed by all authors.
The exploratory analytic studies on which these
patters of dysfunction were based, however, must be interpreted with caution
and can be criticized appropriately for limitations in design (Cummins, 1991;
Hoehn & Baumeister, 1994; Parham & Mailloux, 2001). Because Ayres was
constantly exploring new ideas, she used a different battery of tests in each
study. Therefore, none of the studies was a true replication of a preceding
one. Furthermore, her samples were heterogeneous and consistently small in
number relative to the number of test scores that were analyzed. Terminology
used to describe the factors that emerged in these studies was also
inconsistent; therefore, comparing results from these studies and drawing
conclusions based on their combined contributions are difficult. Nonetheless,
the practical implications of these patterns have been very important for our
understanding of the nature of the sensory integrative dysfunction seen in
children and in interpreting the results of children on the SIPT (Ayres, 1989).
Mulligan (1998) attempted to validate a five factor model of sensory
integrative dysfunction based on the SIPT scores of large, heterogeneous group
of children. The model tested was consistent with current views of patterns of
sensory integrative dysfunction and included a bilateral integration and
sequencingpattern, a somatosensory pattern, and a postural-ocular movement
pattern. Although the results supported the hypothesize model as reasonable, a
number of weaknesses were indentified with it that supported further analyses
of alternative models. One of the most important findings was the very string
relationship among all of a higher-order general factors,
which initially as termed generalized practic dysfunction. (Editors` note:
Mulligan later agree that her higher-order factor may
reflect a general inefficiency of CNS functioning, particularly in the areas or
systems measured by the SIPT. It might be more appropriate to label this
higher-order general factor general sensory integrative dysfunction. [Mulligan, personal communication, June 1, 1999].).
IN view of this research, rather than purport the existence of separate
patterns of dysfunction (related to dysfunction in particular neural
substrates), as previous studies have done, Mulligan suggested that specific
patterns of dysfunction (based on deficient SIPT scores) should be viewed as
extensions of generalized sensory integrative dysfunction (see Chapter 1 for
more specifics on Mulligan’s factor-analytic study)
The idea of a general factor emphasizes the complexity of our CNS and supports
the systems or holistic view of the CNS discussed earlier in this chapter. In
examining construct validity of the SIPT, Lai et al. (1996) provided evidence
that praxis is a unidimensional construct and that both bilateral integration
and sequencing and somatopraxis were a part of this unidimensional construct.
Although their study did not examine all patterns of dysfunction believed to
comprise sensory integrative dysfunction,these result
indicated a shift in thinking regarding the multidimensionality of sensory
integrative dysfunction.
Mulligan (1998) also found that the SIPT was not sufficient to detect problems
related to postural functioning, supporting Fisher and Bundy (1991), who
discussed the importance of using other clinical observations such as examining
equilibrium reactions and antigravity postures to determine postural problems
(see also Chapter 7). Somatopraxis also did not emerge as a separate pattern
(Mulligan, 1998) as it had in previous studies (Ayres, 1966, 1971, 1977; Ayres
et al., 1987). A relationship between sonmatosensory processing and praxis was
evident; however, unlike previous models, which identified somatodyspraxia as a
separate pattern of dysfunction, the relationship between these two areas was
explained by the presence of the general factor (Mulligan, 1998). In view of
this new information, Mulligan suggested that therapists be cautions when
identifying a child as fitting one of the five specific patterns of dysfunction
(based on SIPT scores) previously indentified.
In summary, a multitude of diagnostic labels is used to describe children with
sensory integrative dysfunction, and care must be taken not to use various
terms interchangeably. When conducting research, coexisting diagnoses of study
participants need to be identified and reported so that the effects of
intervention for different types of dysfunction can be examined and considered
in the interpretation of results. Although it appears that the multidimensional
view of sensory integrative dysfunction is shifting to a more unified model, a
consensus on the best way to conceptualize it has not beenreached. Further
understanding of how sensory integrative dysfunction relates with other models
of sensory processing is necessary.
RESEARCH EVALUATING INTERVENTION BASED ON SENSORY INTEGRATION THEORY
In applied research, the definition of intervention based on sensory
integration theory, as an independent variable, has been inconsistent. This is
problematic when one attempts of intervention. Rosenthal and Rosnow (1984)
defined and independent variable as an observable or measurable event
manipulated by a researcher to determine whether there is any effect on another
event (i.e., the dependent variable). Research based on a traditional
definition of sensory integrative- based intervention often requires that the
independent variable or intervention be administered to every individual in
precisely the same manner. Clearly, intervention based on sensory integration
theory defies a simple definition.
Integration based on sensory integration theory is complex because the way in
which it is administered depends on the individual needs of a client
(Ottenbacher, 1991). In addition, the application of intervention as it was
conceptualized classically must be distinguished from more current applications
that involve the use of indirect, consultative models, and the use sensory
integration principles and activities in combination with those from other
frames of reference. Because this distinction is so important, research
evaluating each type, “classic” and “nontraditional”,
will be addressed separately.
Studies of Classic Sensory Integration Intervention.
The classic form of sensory integration intervention tends to be practiced more
often in private, medically orientedclinics than in educational environments.
Such intervention is highly specialized, and it has been recommended that only
therapists who have received advanced training administer it. Kimball (1988,
1999) described several characteristics of sensory integration intervention.
These can be summarized as follows.
* The goal of the intervention is to facilitate appropriate physical and
emotional adaptive responses by improving CNS processing rather than to teach
specific skills.
* Intervention activities are individualized and at the upper levels of the
client`s capacity.
* Intervention is administered by a vigilant practitioner who provides constant
feedback.
* Intervention involves purposeful activities that are client directed and
result in an adaptive response.
* Intervention activities provide enhanced proprioceptive, vestibular, and
tactile sensation.
Although these characteristics provide guidelines for defining the
intervention, they are very broad, and there is room for variability within
each of them. However, interventions that involve the application of sensory
stimulation only, structured group interventions, combined approaches, use of
sensory integration in consultative models, or intervention that applies
sensory integration principles during the practice of specific functional
skills are not considered classic sensory integration intervention. Although
all of these approaches are very appropriate for some children, in a research
context, care must be taken to differentiate them from classic sensory
integration intervention.
In the 1970s and 1980s, many studies were conducted specifically evaluating the
effectiveness of sensory integrationintervention, with mixed results. Many
detailed reviews of this literature are available (Cermak & Henderson,
1989; Hoehn & Baumeister, 1994; Mulligan, 2001; Polatajko et al., 1992,
Vargas & Camilli, 1999). These early studies demonstrated that sensory
integration therapy improved performance in motor, language, and academic areas
(Ayres, 1072a, 1972c; Magrun et al., 1981; White, 1979). Ottenbacher (1982)
performed a meta-analysis of eight intervention effectiveness studies, which
provided support for sensory integration in the remediation of motor, academic,
and language functions, whit the most improvements noted in the motor area.
Specifically in relation to children whit learning disabilities, he reported
that “the average learning disabled students receiving sensory integration
therapy performed better than 75.2 percent of the learning disabled subjects
not receiving therapy” (Ottenbacher, 1982, p.576).
There have not been many group studies in the past 10 years evaluating the
outcomes of sensory integrations interventions in comparison with other
approaches such as tutoring (Wilson et al., 1992) and perceptual- motor
training (Humphries et al., 1992). The more recent studies, however, improved
on previous studies in terms of methodological rigor. These studies concluded that
sensory integration therapy is not any more effective than these traditional
approaches. Polatajko et al. (1992) reviewed seven two- and three- group
experimental studies conducted from 1879 to 1992 using sensory integration
intervention with samples of children with learning disabilities. They
concluded that the results of previous studies do not indicate that sensory
integrationintervention improves the academic performance of children with
learning disabilities more than placebo does. With respect to sensory or motor
performance, the results were inconsistent and indicated overall that sensory
integration intervention may produce minimal positive effects. However, the
generalization of these studies is limited because study subjects included
children with learning disabilities only and because the nature and extent of
the subjects’ sensory integration dysfunction were often not known. No
negative effects sensory integration interventions were ever reported.
Wilson Kaplan (1994) conducted follow up study of the children who had received
either tutoring or sensory integration therapy in their 1992 study. The concluded that the children who received sensory integration
intervention performed better on tests of gross motor function than the
children who received the tutoring intervention. No significant
differences were found on tests that measured reading skills, fine motor
skills, visual motor skills, or behavioral factors. Allen and Donald (1995)
conducted a pilot study to determine the effect of occupational therapy
intervention (based of sensory integration theory) on five children wed
documented sensory integrative dysfunction. There case studies revealed that
four of the five children receiving the therapy improved in the area of motor
performance- the one participant who did not show motor gains was older than
the other children (is age 11 years compared with ages 5 to 8 years).
In a meta-analysis of 32 experimental group studies (16 comparing sensory
integration intervention with other interventions and 16 comparing sensory
integrationintervention with no intervention), Vargas and Camilli (1999)
expressed there main conclusions. First, when compared with a control
situation, the effects of intervention based on sensory integration theory
showed positive results in earlier studies but no differences in later studies.
Second, effect sizes for measures of motor performance and cognition such as IQ
and academic achievement were greater than those for measures of behavior,
language, and sensory and perceptual ability. Third, overall intervention based
on sensory integration theory was as effective as various alternative
intervention approaches.
One way of minimizing or controlling the variation in complex
intervention approach when evaluating it is to reduce the intervention to a
small number of standardized and strictly controlled activities. The
advantages of doing so are that in allows the researcher to examine the
effectiveness of specific components of the intervention while making it easier
to operationally define the independent variable. The main disadvantage in
doing so is probably not very representative of the actual intervention as it
is typically practiced, nor of the construct itself.
Reductionist definitions of sensory integration intervention are common in the
literature (Jarus & Gol, 1995; Ottenbacher et al., 1981; Ottenbacher, 1991;
Well & Smith, 1983). Altough not specifically defined as sensory
integration therapy, De-Gangi et al. (1993) compared a therapist-directed
sensory motor intervention approach. They concluded that the children-directed
approach, which is more characteristic of sensory integration, was better than
the other approach for the development of fine motorskills but less effective
for the improvement of gross motor skills, sensory integration functions and
functional skills. One must take care in interpreting this study because it
measures a certain component of sensory integration intervention (i.e., child
directed versus therapist directed) and not the intervention itself.
Tickle-Degnen and Coster (1995) examined another component of intervention
based on sensory integration theory in the context of intervention sessions.
With the use of videotape, they examined a social element, the nature of
therapist-child interactions during sensory integration intervention. This
social element is one of the most important facets of sensory integration
intervention; Ayres emphasized that achieving positive results depends on the
involvement of a skilled practitioners who can match both input and adaptive
demands to the child`s current needs and capacities. Kaplan et al. (1993) also
suggested that the bond between a therapist and child may account for some of
the positive perceptions regarding intervention outcomes.
In summary, efficacy research of sensory integration in its
“purest” form provides conflicting results. Overall, the results
provide little supporting evidence that the intervention is helpful in the
remediation of learning or academic difficulties of children. There is some
support, however, that it improves sensory processing; improves some behavioral
problems in children; and, most clearly, improves gross motor performance in
children with learning, behavioral, or motor problems. Results also indicate
that intervention based on sensory integration theory is most effective with
children who have been specificallyidentified with sensory integration problems
and that the intervention appears most effective with younger children.
Studies of Nontraditional Sensory Integration Intervention
Although the classic approach is still used with some children, intervention
based on sensory integration theory is more often modified or used in
conjunction with other approaches. For example, practitioners working with
children in educational settings have modified the way in which they use
sensory integration as a frame of reference to meet the requirements of special
education law and to accommodate the specific needs of students. Additionally,
they have tried to join the move of educators from process approaches toward
more direct instruction models and toward inclusive education (i.e., educating
students with special needs in regular classroom environments and within the
context of regular classroom activities). For example, the used of consultative
services has increased over the past 10 to 15 years (Dunn, 1988; Kemmis &
Dunn, 1996) and occupational therapists are applying sensory integration
principles to help students to be successful with classroom activities already
a part of the typical curriculum (Mulligan, 1997).
There is osme empirical evidence supporting the efficacy of consultative models
(Davis & Gavin, 1994; Dunn, 1990; Kemmis & Dunn, 1996), which often
include a component of sensory integration theory. For example, Case- Smith
(1997) found that one of the most important factors in successful interventions
was the practitioner`s ability to reframe a child`s classroom behaviors using
sensory integration theory. Sensory integration was reported to be the primary
frameof reference for five or the 13 children examined in her study.
In addition to the increased use consultation, practitioners often combine
sensory integration interventions with other approaches, including the
provision of adapted equipment, gross and fine motor skill training, self-care
skill training, and parent and teacher support, as part of child`s total
intervention program. Few studies however, have examined the effectiveness of
sensory integration with other approaches. This is not surprising. As with
consultation, it is very difficult to insolate the contributions of the
specific components of combined approaches to the ultimate outcomes of the
intervention.
A promising study by Case-Smith et al 1998)
demonstrated the effectiveness of school-based occupational therapy services on
fine motor skills and other functional outcomes of preschool children with
developmental delays. In this study, 44 children with developmental delays and
20 children without delays were pretested with a number of outcome measures at
the beginning of the school year and posttested at the end of the school year.
The intervention approaches varied depending on the individual needs of each
preschooler. The use of activities characteristic of intervention based on
sensory integration theory was very common (40.4% of sessions emphasized motor
planning and tactile activities; 31.7% emphasized vestibular and propioceptive
activities) as was the use of visuomotor and manipulations activities (81% of
sessions). The results of this study supported the use of sensory integration
techniques in combination with other approaches for improving fine motor
skills, and functional skills ofpreschoolers with developmental delays.
Operationally defining sensory integration as an independent variable has not
become any easier in the past 10 to 15 years. Rather, with the increased use of
sensory integration in consultative models and in combination with other
approaches, it may very well be more difficult to isolate and define than it
was previously. We do, however, have guidelines that can be used (Kimball,
1988, 1999) to help researchers define sensory integration intervention in its
classic form (see also Appendix A). Examining components of intervention is
also a technique that has been used to further our understanding of the effects
of certain qualities of the intervention. Finally, the use of experimental
designs that do not attempt to control the intervention, as in the study design
used by Case-Smith et al. (1998) and supported by Bower and McLellan (1994), appear
to be a useful way to evaluate the effects of complex, multifaceted, and
individualized interventions.
DIRECTIONS FOR FURTHER RESEARCH
A number of scholars in the field (e.g., Cermak & Henderson, 1989; Kaplan
et al., 1993; Mulligan, 1997; Ottenbacher, 1991; Tickle-Degnen, 1988) have
provided direction for future research related to sensory integration theory
and intervention. Research questions range from the basic premises on which the
theory and hypothesized dysfunction are base to the
efficacy of intervention as it is practiced in its varied forms.
For professional consensus to be reached regarding the usefulness of
intervention based on sensory integration theory, it must be better defined and
conceptualized. Basic research that explores the theoretical bases of sensory
integrationincluding the underlying mechanisms on which it is based, is needed (Tickle-Degnen, 1998). Such research is
useful for determining how and why intervention works rather than if it is
effective. Further understanding of the ways in which sensory integration
relates to other information processing models, such as those described in the
education and psychology literature (See Swanson, 1987) and other sensory
processing models (e.g., Dunn. 1997) is also necessary. Such work may someday
allow researchers and theorists to converge on a unified paradigm for the study
of individuals with sensory integrative dysfunction.
Second, we must better understand the individuals for whom intervention is
useful and be better able to describe participants in research samples.
Specific tests of sensory integrative dysfunction a recommended to identify
individuals indentified as having sensory integrative dysfunction should be
used to evaluate intervention based on sensory integration theory. Research
examining specific patterns of sensory integrative dysfunction may also be
helpful in identifying intervention best suited for certain individuals.
Furthermore, coexisting conditions, including other diagnoses and
characteristics of participants in studies, should always be reported so the
influence of multiple variables can be considered in the interpretation of
results.
Third, psychometrically sound tests of sensory integration are necessary for
indentifying appropriate candidates for intervention as well as for measuring
the effects of intervention, supporting research in the area of test
development. Mulligan (1998) suggested that a new, shorter test emphasizing the
identification of praxisproblems and the underlying sensory integration
functions that may be contributing to the praxis problems be considered. In
addiction, further tests of vestibular function and tests that measure sensory
modulation need to be developed because it is believed that these are important
aspects of sensory integration that have not yet been captured adequately
within a standardized tool (Mulligan, 1998) Mulligan also reported that a tool
that provides an overall score of sensory integrative dysfunction would be
helpful to identify clearly whether dysfunction exists and what the overall
severity of the dysfunction is.
The specific goals of intervention based on sensory integration theory and
expectations for areas of gain differ for various individuals. Therefore, the
dependent measures selected should be consistent with these expectations and be
sensitive enough to detect changes. Cermak and Henderson (1989) reported that
research is greatly hampered by the lack of good measures of intervention
effectiveness (see also Chapter 1). Ways to document both the short-and
long-term effects of intervention are needed. In addiction to measures
evaluating motor, language, academic and behavioral outcomes, measures of the
effects of intervention on occupational performance (e.g., play) and
performance components (e.g., attention, organization, and affect) are need
(Cermak & Henderson, 1989). Based on their research examining parental
hopes for therapy outcomes, Cohn et al. (2000) discussed the importance of
including both child-focused measures (i.e., self-regulation, perceived competence,
and social participation) and parent outcomes (i.e., validation of their
child’s problems andcompetency in applying strategies to assist their
children) when measuring the effectiveness of intervention.
Fourth, the complexity and individualized nature of intervention based on
sensory integration must be carefully considered when evaluating the
effectiveness of the intervention. For traditional experimental designs,
sensory integration intervention must be better defined and controlled.
Kimball’s (1988, 1999) description is useful for identifying the basic
and necessary components and characteristics of classic intervention. Specific
questions researchers can ask themselves when operationally defining
interventions have been outlined by Miller and Kinnealey (1993). For example,
researchers must determine how much of the child’s responses. Protocols
that describe the interventions need to develop and reviewed by experts in the
field, and procedural reliability checks should be implemented to ensure the
consistency and accuracy of the application of the protocols. (See also
Appendix A for the STEP-SI protocol developed by Miller and her colleagues).
Reductionistic definitions of intervention based on sensory integration theory,
as note earlier, can also be used to minimize intervention variation and allow
the researcher to insolate and examine the effectiveness of specific components
of this multifaceted approach. However, car must be taken when generalizing the
results of these studies because the sum of the effects of
individual components of an intervention do not necessarily add up to
the total effects of the intervention.
If interventions are based on a consultative model or are used in combination
with other approaches, then all approaches need to beindentified, descried, and
accounted for in the study. It is, however, not always necessary to control the
intervention in order to evaluate it. Case-Smith et al. (1998) demonstrated how
researchers are able to evaluate therapy services being individualized and provided
in natural contexts. Miller and Kinnealey (1993) cited single subject
experimental designs as well as case studies and other qualitative approaches
as being particularly relevant when studying the effects of sensory integrative
interventions. Rather than masking individual differences and attempting to
produce homogeneous groups, these approaches explore the effects of individual
differences, allow for individualized intervention, and are useful in
determining for whom intervention is most helpful. Regardless
of the research method, however, it important for investigators to carefully
describe the delivery of the intervention and monitor its implementation
throughout the research process.
SUMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
Sensory integration as a frame of reference has provide a useful framework for
assisting occupational therapy practitioners, parents, and teachers in
understanding children’s behavior in ways that make sense. Despite
contradictory empirical evidence regarding the effectiveness of this approach over
the years, it has remained very popular. We must not lose sight of the fact
that sensory integration has a great deal in common with more global and
accepted frames of reference such as developmental approaches and information
processing. This commonality with other approaches enhances its credibility and
the comfort level of the practitioners who choose to use it. Sensory
integration as a frame ofreference takes a unique perspective in organizing
largely accepted view regarding the way in which the CNS works, what we know
about child development, and how one learns and process sensation.
Developing consensus regarding the validity of sensory integration will require
a creative synthesis of past and present empirical efforts. Such efforts
require cooperation of multitude of researchers from various backgrounds and
expertise in diverse research methodologies. The science of sensory integration
is still in its infancy, and no single research approach has emerged as the
methodology of choice in establishing empirical consensus. The absence of a
unifying research paradigm is a function of the highly complex subject that
sensory integration is. The exploration and application a multitude of research
approaches should be encouraged and is viewed as a positive development.
As concerned, caring professionals, we must believe in the therapeutic merit of
what we do. At the same time, we must acknowledge the limitations of the
scientific evidence that support the outcomes of the interventions that we
strive to achieve with our clients. There is scientific evidence to support the
use of intervention based on sensory integration theory for children with
sensory processing disorders, and there is scientific evidence to dispute its
use. Careful monitoring of the progress of our clients is, therefore, crucial
because we must remain accountable. Finally, we must all take some level of
responsibility for furthering our knowledge base and our understanding of
sensory integration. As a part of this process, we must value research as an
integral part of practice.